Cadernos de Saúde Pública
ISSN 1678-4464
34 nº.5
Rio de Janeiro, Maio 2018
ARTIGO
Dando significado ao vírus Zika: heterogeneidade nas representações sociais do vírus de acordo com a história de infecção
Gilvan Ramalho Guedes, Raquel Zanatta Coutinho, Leticia Marteleto, Wesley Henrique Silva Pereira, Denise Duarte
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/0102-311X00003217
Zika Vírus; Assunção de Riscos; Apoio Social
Introduction
In late 2015, Zika was making its first cases in the Americas. Although transmitted by the same vector as Dengue and Chikungunya, Zika was gaining attention in the news due to its most alarming consequence: microcephaly, a type of congenital Zika syndrome. The media concentrated its coverage on babies born with small head circumference and a stranded future. The fear of large-scale consequences for a whole generation led authorities to recommend postponing pregnancy and reinforcing the measures of vector elimination that were already in place for dengue 1,2.
The intense broadcast of images showing babies born with microcephaly caused social turmoil and a sense of collective vulnerability. In these initial months of the epidemic, there was uncertainty about causes, consequences and potential for spread, making it difficult for the public to understand this new and quickly evolving public health threat. Despite the dramatic coverage Zika has been given, little is still known about how people are interpreting the disease, particularly whether they understand it as a real threat, how they make sense of their own vulnerability and whether they are putting knowledge into action.
This study examines how individuals collectively signify the Zika virus (ZIKV) epidemic. Based on the idea that personal involvement influences and shapes the representation of diseases 3, we hypothesize that the meanings people give to what they see or hear during the outbreak are subjected to their own experiences with the virus. We call web of meanings the interaction between the meanings surrounding ZIKV atributed by individuals who share the same social context. Individuals infected with ZIKV, Dengue, or Chikungunya might see the disease differently from those whose contact with these viruses is solely through the news or public health campaigns. We perform a social network analysis using novel data on individuals' evocations regarding the ZIKV collected in a municipality that's a hotspot for Dengue in Brazil. We look at three different histories of self-reported infection: those never infected by any of the diseases transmitted by the Aedes aegypti mosquito; those that have been infected by Dengue or Chikungunya, but not by ZIKV, and those previously infected by ZIKV. This is a first step in disentangling the different ways individuals attribute collective meanings to ZIKV as the outbreak unfolds. The analysis will equip policymakers with information on social thinking, a necessary tool for effective public health campaigns targeted at behavioral change towards preventive behavior against the ZIKV threat and its socioeconomic, health and reproductive consequences.
The ZIKV epidemic: roots, diffusion and social behavior during the outbreak
In Brazil, ZIKV was first recorded in early 2015 and the epidemic rapidly gained momentum. In May 2015, the Pan American Health Organization (PAHO) announced the first confirmed case of infection 4. In February 2016, ZIKV became a disease of mandatory notification in Brazil. Since the beginning of the infection until the most recent epidemiological report in 2017 (epidemiological week 15) 223,230 cases have been reported, distributed across over 2,000 municipalities with 133,527 confirmed cases. In 2016, ZIKV's incidence rate was 105.3 cases per each 100,000 inhabitants 5.
Considering that Dengue's infection rates are even higher and both diseases are transmitted by the same vector, ZIKV's potential for spreading is enormous. Data from the epidemiological week 52 of 2016, for example, shows that Dengue's incidence was as high as 733.4 in Brazil 5. One challenge public officials and epidemiologists face when estimating ZIKV epidemic dimension is that as much as 80% of infections are symptomless, increasing the potential for underreport. Furthermore, despite symptoms specific to ZIKV (like rash with itch, joint pain/swelling, and conjunctivitis), others are similar to Dengue (fever, headache, muscle pain and rash) 1,6,7.
The ZIKV epidemic put Brazil in the epicenter of international attention and media coverage 8. Concerns are centered around its most dangerous consequence, microcephaly - an illness that affects the fetus of infected mothers and that is one of the possible outcomes of what is now called congenital Zika syndrome 2,9,10. In 2016, 2,653 cases of microcephaly or alterations of the central nervous system were confirmed in Brazil 11. ZIKV can also lead to developmental, neurological and ophthalmological problems even if a fetus does not develop microcephaly 8,12.
ZIKV is also transmitted via sexual intercourse, blood transfusions and amniotic fluid 12,13,14, requiring protective strategies in addition to mosquito vector elimination. Behavioral changes are the only way to slow down sexual transmission 15, making it imperative that researchers discover how individuals change their behavior in response to the epidemic. Several examples in the epidemiological and social literature regarding social perception and behavior during epidemic are elucidative. A study found that during the H1N1 flu outbreak in 2009, changing behavior was associated with perceptions that the swine flu was severe, that the risk of infection was high and that the outbreak would last for a long time 16. Respondents who reported that the outbreak was “over-hyped” were less likely to demonstrate any change in behavior - a potential scenario for persistent spread. Focusing on the SARS (severe acute respiratory syndrome) epidemic in Hong Kong, another study showed that although the perceived chance of infection was low (9%), the fear of infection was high (48%) and, possibly due to the campaigns, people learned how to efficiently employ measures of protection over time 17.
One key factor associated with changes in social representation is personal involvement. Rouquette 18 describes that the amount of personal involvement will vary according to the evaluation of risk of infection, the personal exposure to risk and the perceived capacity to act towards it. The higher the three factors, the higher the involvement. Thus, a change in personal involvement triggers a change in some of the aspects of its social representation 18 and therefore the understanding of a social phenomenon. As such, the social perception of health threats like ZIKV are likely sensitive to the degree of exposure and involvement, triggering different coping mechanisms and behavioral responses 3,19. For this reason, finding out the meanings individuals give to infections, their risk perceptions, and whether they differ according to personal experience, particularly for emerging threats for which individual behaviors towards them are largely unknown, is an important instrument for more effective public health communications and for the promotion of protective behavior amidst these epidemic.
Methods
This study used novel data on mental representations of ZIKV for urban residents of Governador Valadares, Minas Gerais State, Brazil. The choice for Governador Valadares is justified because the city ranks third in the state for the LIRAa index (Assessment of Aedes aegypti Infestation Index), and is classified in the very high incidence cluster for Dengue in Brazil 20. The 2017 LIRAa for Governador Valadares was 8.5%, above the minimum threshold of 3.9% for the high risk category 21. In 2016 alone, 1,609 cases of Dengue, 115 of Chikungunya, and 3,154 of ZIKV were reported in Governador Valadares. Key for our study, the city was also hit by contaminated mud from the Samarco dam failure in November 2015 22. This unprecedented incident created severe water shortages due to the contamination of the city's main river by heavy metals, leading residents to stock water at home. Such massive amounts of stocked water contributed to the proliferation of the A. aegypti in the region, making the city a key social context to understand ZIKV's social representation.
The selection of respondents was based on a baseline probabilistic survey conducted from January 2014 to June 2016 with 1,226 individuals. The baseline survey is part of the research project Migration, Vulnerability, and Environmental Change in the Rio Doce Valley. For the present study, we aimed at collecting 150 respondents
Table 1 Distribution of interviews by gender, socioeconomic status, and level of exposure to the Zika virus. Governador Valadares, Minas Gerais State, Brazil, 2016.
|
Although information on infection was self-reported, we performed an initial screening to reduce potential bias. During the first phone contact, respondents were asked how they knew if they were infected or not by Dengue, Chikungunya, or ZIKV. We started by letting them freely suggest the diagnostic criterion. In all cases of self-diagnosis, the phone call contact was interrupted and a new call was made to the next pearson appearing in the list of random numbers from the baseline survey. This procedure was repeated until we found a person whose diagnosis had been made by a health professional. For those who said s/he was diagnosed by a health professional, we further asked if they had taken a serological test for detection or if the health professional clinically diagnosed him/her.
The questionnaire used in this study comprises of two modules: one for the collection of data on representations of the ZIKV and another on personal involvement with the disease. Based on the Free Words Association Technique 25, the first module contains the following questions: (1) “When I say ‘ZIKA VIRUS' tell me the first five words or expressions that come into your mind, without thinking about them”; (2) “Now, I would like you to put them in order of importance for you, 1 meaning the most and 5 the least important”; (3) “You mentioned that expression ‘FILL IN' was the most important for you. What does it mean to you? [Open question]”; (4) “Why is it the most important to you?”.
The second module contains basic information regarding whether respondents had been previously diagnosed by a health professional with each of the three main diseases, the type of diagnostic (clinical or serological), time since diagnosed and if any member in the household or close friend were diagnosed with any of the diseases.
Evocations were coded according to their meaning, a procedure described elsewhere (see Supplementary Material for details on coding: http://cadernos.ensp.fiocruz.br/csp/public_site/arquivo/supplementary-material-to-the-manuscript_4890.pdf)26. Although 240 unique words or expressions were used, their grouping into common concepts resulted in 66 categories of meanings. We applied inductive reasoning when defining preliminary codes to reflect the participant's meanings. These codes reduced the amount of variation without losing each usage's unique meaning. When the word was too vague for proper classification, the researcher read the answers to question 3 searching for an explanation that would satisfy the meaning given to the vague word. In cases when it did not fit a pre-defined code, a new code would be created.
These coded evocations were modeled using social network analysis 27. Other proposed techniques for this type of data 25,28 lack the ability to fully quantify topological characteristics of the web of meanings, such as the centrality of certain evocations and the density of the total network and its communities. Furthermore, identification of subgroups of individuals based on their shared meanings is arbitrary in other techniques, while validated through simulation within the network analysis framework 29. We started by transforming the coded evocations into relational data using the R package wordevok we developed (The R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria; http://www.r-project.org). The following assumptions were used to create data on linked evocations.
Assumption 1 [personal cognitive network]: evocations from the same individual j, vk(j), are mentally connected.
Since evocations gravitate around an inducting term, the likelihood of random responses is reduced. However, because people rank words by their perceived order of importance, this rank may provide information on their practical values 3. The following assumptions account for that:
Assumption 2 [distance]: for the same individual j, any two evocations contained in the evocation vector V(j) are more important the closer they sit to each other.
Assumption 3 [order]: for the same individual j, any two evocations of equal distance are more important the higher the order of the origin evocation. Thus, for an edge formed by two evocations, vk(j) and vz(j): w(ekz(j) = vok(j), vo+1z(j)) > w(ekz(j) = vo+1k(j), vo+2z(j)) > ... > w(ekz(j) = vo+q-1k(j), vo+qz(j)), with vok(j) being the k-th vertex of the j-th individual of order o, and q the length of V(j).
Assumption 2 assures that two related evocations would have more significance the shorter the distance they have from each other. Assumption 3 assures that in addition to the distance, the order that the origin evocation is located matters for weighting purposes. If two evocations have the same distance (words 1 and 2 or words 4 and 5, for example), the edge with the highest order origin evocation would be assigned the largest weight. Although evocations are referred to as origin and destination, the network is still undirected.
To parametrize these weights we defined two parameters, π and ρ, that penalize edges by distance and order, respectively. We propose a general formula to account for Assumptions 2 and 3. Parameters ρ and θ are defined as ρ(i,h) = m - |i - h| and θ(i,h) = 22m+2-(i+h) - 1. Both parameters are indexed on i and h - the rank of evocations within the evocation vector, V(j). The general formula for the edge weights is, then:
where the τ factor corresponds to the normalized weight for each edge, and ω represents a factor that penalizes each person's evocation vector by its length. Parameters m and M are the vector length related to the j-th individual and the largest vector in the sample, respectively.
When evocations are coded, loops are likely to occur. These loops, however, are not natural to the network structure, but induced by the coding process. Imagine the following evocation vector for the j-th person: V(j) = {Disease, Dengue, Mosquito, Aedes aegypti, Microcephaly}. If coding produces the vector Vs(j) = {Disease, Dengue, Mosquito, Mosquito, Microcephaly}, the edge evs3Mosquito(j), vs4Mosquito(j) represents an induced loop. To eliminate such loops, two strategies were used:
1. Reclassification of identical evocations for the same individual in the entire dataset, so they are represented differently.
2. When reclassification was not possible, the vertex of smaller order was dropped and the vertex set rearranged accordingly. For instance, if Vs(j) = {v(so)k(j), v(so+1)z(j), v(so+2)t(j), v(so+3)x(j), v(so+4)x(j)}, it was redefined as Vs(j) = {v(so)k(j), v(so+1)z(j), v(so+2)t(j), v(so+3)x(j)}.
With the relational data in hand, the web of meanings regarding the ZIKV was decomposed using the algorithm proposed by Newman & Girvan 29. This algorithm optimizes the modularity of a partition, comparing the observed network with a network with the same observed degree distribution but with edges placed at random. The decomposition was performed for the full sample and for each history of infection
Figure 1 Web of meanings for Zika Virus by history of infection. Governador Valadares, Minas Gerais State, Brazil, 2016.
|
Results
Heterogeneity in representations of the ZIKV
We start by describing the web of meanings formed by the full sample and by each history of infection, exploring the meanings given by respondents to the words with highest centrality
Figure 2 Networks of meanings by self-reported diagnosis criterion among individuals infected by the Zika virus. Governador Valadares, Minas Gerais State, Brazil, 2016.
|
Full network (density = 0.202; nodes = 69; edges = 475)
The complete series of evocations form a web of meaning
The fourth word, “malaise”, is another consistently mentioned symptom. The fifth and sixth words, “mosquito” and “fear”, express the feeling of being infected and having to deal with either the symptoms or “getting pregnant and having a child with problems”, given the surge of ZIKV that appeared in Governador Valadares during the months prior to this research. Fear also relates to the gravity of the disease, who noted that “it can kill”, “it's difficult to prevent”, “it's serious or else the media wouldn't be talking about it all the time”, and “everybody is at risk”.
“Something bad”, the seventh expression, encompasses an array of words related to the epidemic (e.g. bad, horrible, discouraging), a superficial description that did not portray severity or chaos. The eighth word, “weakness”, also represents a collection of symptoms related to the lethargy associated with the disease (e.g. tired, depressive, weak, only wants to lie down, sleepy). “Death”, the ninth word, represents the worst possible consequence. Since cases of death by ZIKV are rather rare, we suspect that those who mention this level of severity are unfamiliar with the disease. “Microcephaly”, the most broadcasted word by the media, appears only in the tenth position.
In sum, these ten most central words'meanings are very diverse and related to the threat posed by ZIKV, its potential for spread and the lack of commitment to make improvements that could stop its spread. Five sub-networks (clusters) of meanings emerge from inside the general network. We named these clusters after their latent meaning according to their words with highest centralities
The first cluster, lack of social and personal responsibility, is mainly formed by the central words disease, mosquito, standing water, death, lack of prevention, something bad, trash, prevention, epidemics, danger. Respondents in this cluster acknowledge that the disease will not be eradicated until the local population does their share, cleaning their houses and backyards, but also until the government takes responsibility over public places.
The symptoms cluster contained words related to the disease's most common symptoms: pain, malaise, weakness, fever, rash, and depression. The media terror cluster, except for the word microcephaly, is formed by words of low centrality in the general network, but that help to form the image of a tumultuous situation. They are: chaos, suffering, calamity, fear, medicine, disabilities, badly treated at the clinic, public health, cost of treatment, vaccine, abortion, and family. During the ZIKV outbreak, the urgent care system was overloaded and the health crisis was making headlines.
The public health cluster is very similar to the first, but gives extra weight to the population's lack of commitment (first word). The other words are health, awareness, symptoms, population's fault, treatment, law, and lack of treatment. The fifth cluster resembles the media terror cluster, but has a simpler topology since it only includes the words fear, pregnancy, and problem. From all those clusters, this is likely the one from which the only thing learned about ZIKV is its relationship with microcephaly, without any other lesson learned about preventable diseases, public health, or even people to blame. We call it microcephaly.
History of infection
To disentangle the effects of respondents' history of infection, we separated people's evocations who have had ZIKV from those who have had other mosquito-borne disease and those who have not had any diseases transmitted by the A. aegypti. Then, three new networks of representations surrounding ZIKV were created
Individuals never infected (network density = 0.232; nodes = 55; edges = 344)
The web of meanings for individuals who have never been infected with any of the three infectious diseases is composed of many words and clusters, much like the analysis of the full network. The ten words with the highest level of centrality are: disease, death, weakness, mosquito, pain, prevention, something bad, lack of prevention, fever, and trash. Microcephaly has a centrality value of 31.5 from a 0 to 239.6 centrality range. Note how something bad and death, words that are unspecific and demonstrate unfamiliarity with the arbovirus, are present in this network.
Individuals infected with Dengue and Chikungunya (network density = 0.171; nodes = 43; edges = 154)
The web of meaning becomes more restricted and structured as the history of infection increases
Individuals infected with ZIKV (network density = 0.159; nodes = 37; edges = 106)
The web of meaning of those infected with ZIKV is highly restricted. The most central words are related to the disease's symptoms, just like the cluster symptoms of the general network, with massive weight given to the word pain, which now becomes the most central word in the network
As an attempt to investigate whether networks of meanings would change according to the diagnostic criterion for people with positive history of ZIKV infection, we stratified the sample
Discussion
Our findings suggest important heterogeneity regarding the collective representation of ZIKV. Our decomposition revealed five sub-networks of meanings, each embracing a different nuance of the epidemic: lack of social and personal responsibility, symptoms, overblown media coverage terror, public health, and microcephaly.
We find that personal involvement with ZIKV is associated with different ways to make sense of the epidemic. Using a proxy for personal involvement as history of infection, we established a model to represent the network of meanings regarding ZIKV and its associated social context, including its symptoms, fears, causes, and consequences. Using novel data on social representation of ZIKV epidemic applied to a network model for a Brazilian municipality with high prevalence of Dengue and A. aegypti infestation, we found evidence that history of infection and diagnostic accuracy are key to relabeling ZIKV's social representation .
Respondents who have never been infected have a much more diverse network of meanings related to the interpretation of ZIKV, one that is very similar to the configuration of the general network. Because the most important idea surrounding those representations relates to disease and death, it is possible that these maps of meanings are being shaped by the everyday messages spread by the media, which focus on preventive measures against mosquitos and the risk of microcephaly. As a result, people with no history of infection (or those with a higher likelihood of being false negatives) are more likely to think of the epidemic in a narrower way. This is a particularly interesting finding, suggesting that lay thinking is more predominant among those with less intimacy with the social object, being more easily affected by external information sources such as the media.
As individuals' history of infection evolves, they start to recognize the importance of prevention promoted by public authorities and their own share of responsibility in preventing infection. They also demonstrate more fear of getting infected and of its consequences, such as microcephaly. Their network seems more realistic as it encompasses meanings related to the symptoms, lack of personal and social responsibility, and how the disease can be eradicated. Those who have been infected with ZIKV have a more objective interpretation of the virus, especially those diagnosed through serological testing.
Given the diversity of meanings surrounding ZIKV, trying to reach the entire population using only one message or strategy might not be an effective way to reach individuals most able or ready to change. Until now, ZIKV public health campaigns have followed campaigns similar to Dengue, focusing on eliminating standing water and of mosquito breeding sites 32 More effective campaigns could benefit from a deeper understanding of the needs and perceptions of its intended audience(s), including their “beliefs, current actions, and social and physical environment” 33 (p. 5). Our results suggest that those who have been infected are the key actors for effective communication within the community. While people who have had Dengue can inform the general population about ways to prevent the proliferation of the A. aegypti, those who have been infected with ZIKV can share their experience of facing ZIKV's harsh symptoms.
One example of how this could help clarify existing campaigns in Governador Valadares is the fact that according to the most recent LIRAa report for the city, most mosquito larvae were found inside residential homes (36.7% in drains and 35.3% in water tanks on the ground). In face of these results, policymakers would erroneously think that Governador Valadares inhabitants do not know about their responsibility in preventing an epidemic. When we look at the data, however, we learn that these same spots are present in the persons' networks of meanings regarding ZIKV. Since individuals are usually well informed about what is necessary to achieve change, barriers to effective behavioral responses might lay elsewhere.
While extending the literature in multiple ways, some limitations remain. It is possible that female respondents are better informed about the virus because of its immediate impact on pregnancy. There is also evidence that those with higher socioeconomic status are better equipped than their low socioeconomic status counterparts to prevent ZIKV infection and are less vulnerable to the conditions that increase the risk of infection 34. Moreover, because infestation of A. aegypti varies regionally across the country, the risk of infection and the likelihood of knowing someone infected likely differ geographically, with potential implications for the collective representation of the disease and for differences in behavioral responses. Future work should therefore extend this study's findings by examining how gender, geographical location, and socioeconomic status interact with the understandings of the social representation of the ZIKV epidemic.
Second, observational and laboratory studies suggest that involvement with the risk of infection changes individuals' perceptions and increases the likelihood of protective behavior 35,36,37. Thus, it is likely that knowing or caring for someone who has been infected with ZIKV might change perceptions about the epidemic, regardless of their own infection status. Intensity, duration, and number of symptoms may also affect the personal experiences with the disease and must be incorporated in future analysis. We also plan to extend this analysis by accounting for exposure within households and ego-centered networks of individuals with different histories of infection. Indirect involvement may be an appropriate substitute for experience and knowledge of the disease 38.
Third, the personal histories of infections we used were self-reported and stratified by different self-reported diagnostic criteria. Many factors, including hypochondria, lack of symptoms, and memory bias in patients with multiple morbidities, could affect the likelihood of false positives and false negatives. Yet, bias is more likely to remain among those with no history of infection than among those who have been infected, since all diseases here considered can be symptomless. More work is needed to understand the extent of these potential bias factors by comparing our results with the networks of meanings among patients screened at health clinics, using actual serological tests and clinical procedures conducted by trained health professionals.
Despite the limitations associated with self-reporting, gathering self-reported information on diagnosis has some advantages. First, there is a robust correlation between self-reported and biomarker-based health status, even for health indicators that are inherently more subjective 24,35. Second, by stratifying the analysis by history of infection and diagnostic criterion allowed us to identify when representations were more reflective of the expected clinical symptoms of the diseases. Given the difficulty in accessing medical information, our analysis is a necessary step to shed light on the heterogeneity in which individuals signify ZIKV during an epidemic using survey data and, as such, help to improve health communication messages to reach a wider audience with more precision.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the Center for Regional Development and Planning (CEDEPLAR), Federal University of Minas Gerais, the Population Research Center, University of Texas at Austin, and the Population Studies Center, University of Pennsylvania for institutional support (including English proofreading). We would also like to thank the Brazilian Graduate Studies Coordinating Board (Capes) for financial support of two of the authors, and a post-doctoral schoolarship for one of the authors. We also thank Leonardo Gomes Sousa for discussion on how to better communicate and represent results using the Gephi software. To Brazilian National Research Council (CNPq; 431872/2016-3), Minas Gerais State Research Foundation (FAPEMIG; APG-01553-16); Rede Clima and Capes for the financial support.
References
Cadernos de Saúde Pública | Reports in Public Health
Rua Leopoldo Bulhões 1480 - Rio de Janeiro RJ 21041-210 Brasil
Secretaria Editorial +55 21 2598-2511.
cadernos@fiocruz.br